Shopping Value and Satisfaction
By: balbisuasti • December 14, 2012 • Essay • 815 Words (4 Pages) • 1,500 Views
2. Literature review and hypotheses
2.1. Shopping value and satisfaction
Satisfaction judgments are thought to be comprised of both
affective (Mano and Oliver, 1993; Westbrook, 1987) and cognitive
elements (Oliver, 1993; Oliver and Swan, 1989). Theoretically,
this notion is embodied in the "two-appraisal" model of
satisfaction evaluation (Oliver, 1989; Weiner, 1986), which
posits that affective responses arise from evaluation of the
outcomes of product/service usage, followed by cognitive interpretation
and related processes (e.g., expectancy-disconfirmation)
which lead to satisfaction. Summary satisfaction judgments,
such as satisfaction with the retailer, are then thought to rely on
the accumulated affective experiences with a product or service,
and beliefs and other cognitions which are retained and updated
over time (Oliver, 1997; Mittal et al., 1999; Westbrook, 1987).
A related theory of needs satisfaction suggests that all
consumption events are capable of fulfilling needs at lower,
more functional levels, as well as higher, more psychological
levels (Oliver, 1997; Herzberg et al., 1959). More tangible attributes
of goods and services are thought to be "bivalent satisfiers,"
or those characteristics which can both satisfy and dissatisfy
consumers. Bivalent satisfiers provide input to the cognitive
processes (e.g., expectancy-disconfirmation) underlying satisfaction
(Oliver, 1997), and should be closely related to assessments
of utilitarian value. More abstract, higher-order characteristics of
goods and services are considered "monovalent satisfiers," or
those "extras" which can only contribute to satisfaction – and not
dissatisfaction – in an affective and psychological manner. Hedonic
value therefore should be closely related to the monovalent
satisfiers in the retail environment.
Given this background, it is reasonable to expect that perceptions
of the value of a retail experience should be important
components of the two fundamental satisfaction appraisals, and
hence should be important to overall satisfaction judgments. As a
reflection of this, retail studies have shown linkages between hedonic
and utilitarian value and satisfaction (Babin et al., 1994;
Babin et al., 2005). Therefore, we should expect the following
relationships.
H1a. Hedonic shopping value will positively influence overall
satisfaction with the retailer.
H1b. Utilitarian shopping value will positively influence overall
satisfaction with the retailer.
However, theory and the nature of the shopping experience
suggest that satisfaction should be more strongly related to hedonic
rather than utilitarian value specifically in the shopping
context. This is so because satisfaction processes are thought to
be dependent in part on the context of consumption (Fournier
and Mick, 1999), and in hedonically valenced contexts research
has made close linkages between emotional responses and satisfaction
(Dawson et al., 1990; Wakefield and Baker, 1998).
Prior research illustrates the multisensory, experiential, and
emotional aspects of shopping (Arnold and Reynolds, 2003;
Westbrook and Black, 1985), and that the in-store experience
can elicit feelings of fun, fantasy fulfillment, escapism, and
excitement (Babin et al., 1994; Wakefield and Baker, 1998).
Hence, while shopping often contains a utilitarian component,
its potential for eliciting emotional response is substantial.
This is important because research has shown that, in experiential
contexts, affect is a key driver of satisfaction. For example,
Price, Arnould, and Tierney (1995) investigated satisfaction
drivers in affectively charged service encounters (white water
rafting) and found that positive affect was highly correlated with
satisfaction while overall performance was not correlated at all.
Similarly, Oliver, Rust and Varki (1997) show that a model of
delight
...